Skip to main content

Gone to the Dogs: Wild Speculations


Imagery, Memory, and Domestication in Human Evolution

Human cognition and behaviour are shaped by intricate relationships between sensory perception, memory, and social dynamics. Two seemingly distinct areas—how individuals process pain and how domestication influences behavioural traits like tameness and aggression—offer profound insights into both our neurological makeup and evolutionary history. By examining the role of pain and emotional imagery alongside domestication theories, we can better understand how these factors may have shaped human development and the domestication of animals. 








Mental Imagery: The Vivid, Muted, Emotional and Painful 

Imagery, the mental ability to visualise and recall sensory or emotional experiences, varies widely among individuals and even species. These variations influence behaviours ranging from empathy to aggression, with implications for how humans relate to pain and emotion—both in themselves and in others.

  • Muted Pain and Emotional Imagery in Psychopathy: Psychopathy often involves reduced or absent emotional recall, with a similar dampening effect on pain imagery. This can result in a muted response to others’ suffering, decreased emotional resonance, and heightened resilience to distress. Could this be related to cases of SDAM (severely deficient autobiographical memory) regarding different types of imagery systems, as seen in those with aphantasia? 
  • Heightened Pain and Emotional Imagery in Hyperempathy: Hyperempathetic individuals experience amplified recall of emotional and physical pain, often to the point of vividly reliving distressing experiences. This heightened imagery fosters compassion and connection but can overwhelm, leading to avoidance or anxiety.

These extremes in sensory and emotional recall may reflect adaptive responses to different environments or social demands within the mental imagery system, creating conditions such as aphantasia, hyperphantasia and other neurodivergent traits. In some scenarios, a muted response to pain and distress could provide a survival advantage, while vivid recall and hyper-responsiveness might enhance social bonding in cooperative settings. 

Fox Domestication: A Tale of Two Behaviours

The domestication experiments on silver foxes, initiated by Dmitry Belyaev, demonstrated how selective breeding for tameness or aggression could drastically alter behaviour in just a few generations. Tame foxes became affiliative, curious, and less fearful, while aggressive foxes displayed heightened stress responses and reduced sociability.

Interestingly, these behavioural shifts were not underpinned by large, singular genetic changes, nor even changes in brain anatomy—both domesticated tame and domesticated aggressive foxes do display a larger cerebral cortex compared to wildtype controls, but this doesn't explain the extreme differences in their behaviours. Instead, these behavioural shifts appear to arise from subtle interactions between genetics and the environment, highlighting the malleability of behaviour through evolutionary pressure.

To add to this, the offspring of tame and aggressive pairings would be either tame or aggressive themselves, there was no blending of traits, reflecting the observations seen in narcissistic and codependent families.

Could the behavioural differences between tame and aggressive foxes mirror the sensory and emotional divergences in humans? For example:
  1. Dampened Emotional and Pain Imagery in Aggressive Foxes: Aggressive foxes may share parallels with psychopathy, displaying reduced affiliative behaviours and muted responses to social or physical stressors.
  2. Heightened Emotional and Pain Imagery in Tame Foxes: Tame foxes might exhibit traits akin to hyperempathy, including stronger social bonds, increased responsiveness to emotional cues, and potentially even heightened pain sensitivity.

These comparisons suggest a continuum of behavioural traits influenced by evolutionary pressures, both in humans and in domesticated animals.

Behavioural Changes Without Genetic Dramas

The fox experiment’s most striking insight was that behavioural shifts—whether towards tameness or aggression—occurred without significant genetic upheaval. This mirrors findings in humans, where behavioural conditions like psychopathy or hyperempathy cannot be identified with specific genetic interactions, but instead appear to stem from complex interactions between genes, environment, and neurobiology rather than singular genetic mutations, or even defined polygenic interactions. 

  • Aggressive Foxes and Reduced Sensory Imagery: Aggressive foxes often showed heightened stress responses but limited affiliative or social behaviours. This muted emotional engagement could reflect a survival strategy akin to reduced pain imagery in psychopathy.
  • Tame Foxes and Amplified Sensory Imagery: Tame foxes displayed exploratory behaviours, curiosity, and strong social bonds, paralleling heightened emotional and pain imagery in hyperempathy.

These parallels provoke questions about the evolutionary utility of such traits. Could tame foxes, like hyperempathetic humans, have benefited from heightened sensitivity to others’ pain and emotions, fostering tighter group dynamics? Conversely, did aggressive foxes, like individuals with muted sensory recall, gain advantages in competitive or high-risk environments?

What mental imagery could silver foxes possess and does it differ between these experimental groups?

Reflections on Domestication and Imagery

Humans, too, have undergone a form of self-domestication, with societal pressures favouring cooperation, reduced aggression, and increased social bonding. Yet, just as in Belyaev’s foxes, the genetic underpinnings of these behavioural shifts are subtle and dispersed.

The parallels between human sensory imagery and fox behaviour invite deeper exploration:

  1. Dampened Imagery: Could reduced pain and emotional recall in psychopathy or aggressive foxes reflect adaptive strategies for competitive or survival-driven contexts?
  2. Heightened Imagery: Might amplified sensory recall in hyperempathy or tame foxes represent evolutionary adaptations favouring cooperation, care, and social cohesion?
  3. Pain Perception and Behaviour: How closely are emotional and physical pain linked to behavioural traits like tameness or aggression across species?

The behavioural and sensory spectrum observed in humans and animals offers valuable insights into the forces that shape evolution. As the fox domestication experiments demonstrate, behaviour is not dictated by genetic simplicity but emerges from a complex interplay of biology, environment, and experience.

Conclusion

This exploration of pain and emotional imagery, memory, and domestication sheds light on the potential intricate interplay of mental processes, behaviours, and evolutionary pressures. While speculative, these musings highlight how the ways we perceive and remember pain might have shaped human behaviours, particularly our capacity for empathy and social bonding.

Drawing parallels between human evolution and animal domestication is a fascinating but limited exercise. The analogy underscores how selective pressures, whether for tameness or aggression, shape behaviours across species, but human evolution is far more complex. Cultural, social, and individual agency add layers that transcend genetic or environmental determinants seen in fox domestication.

The concept of "domesticating" humanity, however, is both compelling and unsettling. What might happen if selective pressures, whether biological or societal, were intentionally applied to amplify or diminish traits like empathy, aggression, or resilience? With advances in genetic engineering and technology, this question is no longer purely theoretical but one that carries profound ethical implications.

As we face a transhuman future, these questions demand careful consideration. Should we selectively enhance traits like empathy or resilience at the risk of diminishing diversity? Could we unintentionally create extremes that undermine the balance essential for human coexistence? These dilemmas are not just speculative but pressing, as the tools to shape our evolution are increasingly within reach.

Ultimately, this reflection offers more questions than answers, encouraging us to consider not only the science of behaviour but also the ethics of intervention. As we navigate the challenges of shaping humanity’s future, we must remember that diversity, complexity, and unpredictability have always been at the heart of human success. Preserving these qualities may be as crucial to our evolution as any trait we seek to enhance.

What are the true consequences of selectively shaping human traits?


Comments

  1. Diary of a CEO - The Narcissism Doctor: "1 In 6 People Are Narcissists!"

    https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=hTkKXDvSJvo

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. Ted Talks - The gentle power of highly sensitive people | Elena Herdieckerhoff

      20–30% of the population is estimated to be highly sensitive people (HSPs)

      https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=pi4JOlMSWjo





      Delete
    2. Does primary narcissism exist in newborn babies? Evidence from sleep science
      https://www.frontiersin.org/journals/psychology/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00330/full

      Delete
    3. Ego tripping: Why do psychedelics "enlighten" some people — and make others giant narcissists? https://www.salon.com/2024/03/08/ego-tripping-why-do-psychedelics-enlighten-some-people--and-make-others-giant-narcissists/

      Delete
    4. The narcissistic scientist: big brain, big head? – Science Weekly podcast https://www.theguardian.com/science/audio/2017/jan/25/the-narcissistic-scientist-big-brain-big-head-science-weekly-podcast

      Delete
    5. An Essay on Science and Narcissism: How do high-ego personalities drive research in life sciences? https://www.researchgate.net/publication/301658739_An_Essay_on_Science_and_Narcissism_How_do_high-ego_personalities_drive_research_in_life_sciences

      Delete
  2. Two types of aggression in human evolution https://www.pnas.org/doi/10.1073/pnas.1713611115

    Aggressive and psychopathic traits are linked to the acquisition of stable but imprecise hostile expectations
    https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC10256845/

    Hypotheses for the Evolution of Reduced Reactive Aggression in the Context of Human Self-Domestication
    https://www.frontiersin.org/journals/psychology/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01914/full

    Self-domesticated by violence to be peaceful. And violent
    https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1059712319878887?icid=int.sj-abstract.similar-articles.4

    ReplyDelete
  3. Towards a comparative science of emotion: Affect and consciousness in humans and animals
    https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0149763419303677

    Do animals live in the present?: Current evidence and implications for welfare
    https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0168159108000142

    "people with aphantasia are more present in the moment because they have difficulty visualizing their experiences. Aphantasia is a condition that affects about 2–4% of the population and makes it difficult for people to visualize their experiences"

    ReplyDelete
  4. Kind granddaughters of angry grandmothers: The effect of domestication on vocalization in cross-bred silver foxes. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2009-09278-003

    ReplyDelete
  5. Why empathy should be your top leadership priority
    Employees crave genuine understanding from their leaders—here’s how to deliver it effectively in 2025.

    https://www.fastcompany.com/91243275/why-empathy-should-be-your-top-leadership-priority

    ReplyDelete

Post a Comment

Popular posts from this blog

A Marriage of Science & Mysticism

Update Feb 2025:  There is now a dedicated space for this type of discussion off of my poetry blog and over over at:   mentalimageryresistance.co.uk   Hopefully this clarified advocacy paper makes more sense to people than my ramblings here:  Locating Unifying Language: Multiple Intelligences, Mental Sensory Perception, and Mental Imagery . For the Record:  I am no authority on this matter. I am an independent researcher and advocate for more clarity in the language and definitions used in mental imagery research. Raising awareness of the need for more research into what those with multisensory aphantasia possess .  Dysikonesia, Aphantasia and a Sophisma Uncovering a Key to the Minds Mental Mixing Deck It occurred to me one day, that taken for their literal meaning only, the psychic concepts of the  Seven Clair's and the Daimon, provide handy language framework for all the main mental senses. Clairvoyance,  literally means ' clear seeing ' or th...

Fifteen Types of Mental Imagery and Vividness Ranges

Update Feb 2025:  There is now a dedicated space for this type of discussion off of my poetry blog and over over at:   mentalimageryresistance.co.uk   Hopefully this clarified advocacy paper makes more sense to people than my ramblings here:  Locating Unifying Language: Multiple Intelligences, Mental Sensory Perception, and Mental Imagery .  For the Record:  I am no authority on this matter. I am an independent researcher and advocate for more clarity in the language and definitions used in mental imagery research. Raising awareness of the need for more research into what those with multisensory aphantasia possess .  Introduction to the Proposed Framework The new framework that I have presented, designed to help with understanding mental imagery, now includes fifteen distinct modalities ( what I call 'yedas'—a long story short, my mind worked backwards from the need to find language for the imagery I do possess—despite having global aphantasia—to the ...

A Transgenic Note

Transgenic mutants  are deliberately deformed, disabled and disordered for animal research purposes. We can create human-animal chimeras, put human DNA into every cell of a mouse, make cells glow green with jellyfish protein, just to be sure, but no amount of human DNA in a mouse will turn it into a human. No matter how much that mouse thinks it might be able to identify with minute aspects of being human, it cannot. It will always be a mutant of a mouse, exploited for biotech gain. Humanized mice do not get human rights.  Law and language for transgenic animals and other such biotech mutants is important and nomenclature is complex . The breeding, movement and welfare of animals under such procedures must be monitored and regulated for the welfare and integrity of both wild and lab lines. Law and language applies differently to animals outside of laboratories, for good reason. Transgenic mutants are covered by the basic animal welfare laws, but they also have ASPA (1986), t...

Anauralia and Anendophasia

"In the silent tenebrous abyss, feelings ebb and flow. Thoughts swim like fishes, in the eternal depth below the surface of my mind, where knowing likes to grow " - Impossible Language .

The Mind’s Eye: A Conversation with Dr. Adam Zeman

Update Feb 2025:  There is now a dedicated space for this type of discussion off of my poetry blog and over over at:   mentalimageryresistance.co.uk   Hopefully this clarified advocacy paper makes more sense to people than my ramblings here:  Locating Unifying Language: Multiple Intelligences, Mental Sensory Perception, and Mental Imagery . For the Record:  I am no authority on this matter. I am an independent researcher and advocate for more clarity in the language and definitions used in mental imagery research. Raising awareness of the need for more research into what those with multisensory aphantasia possess .  Dr. Adam Zeman is a pioneering figure in the study of aphantasia, the inability to produce mental imagery. In a recent discussion with him, I explored the evolving understanding of this fascinating condition, its terminology, and the spectrum of experiences associated with it. Dr. Zeman defines aphantasia as the lack of mental imagery . Initial...